Nortaneous wrote: ↑Sun Feb 21, 2021 6:52 pm
Travis B. wrote: ↑Sun Feb 21, 2021 5:55 pm
Qwynegold wrote: ↑Sun Feb 21, 2021 5:34 pm
Wiktionary says it's /ˈfɝi/. But phonetically it would be something different?
I would not necessarily trust dictionaries; after all I've seen supposedly reliable dictionaries give cot-caught-merged "American" pronunciations despite the fact that a majority of Americans are cot-caught unmerged.
In this case I think a better way to treat "furry" is as /ˈfɜri/, with /ɜ/ being a vowel phoneme that only exists before /r/ in rhotic varieties and with an intervocalic glide /r/ rather than having a rhotic vowel existing in hiatus.
What's objectionable about a rhotic vowel existing in hiatus? I think it's just a case of the lax-tense system breaking down - we have contrasts between /i ij/ and /u uw/, but not /ɝ ɝr/. Like the "high tense vowels" FLEECE and GOOSE ( = /ij uw/), /ɝ/ can occur in hiatus; like the lax vowels KIT and FOOT, it can appear before coda /l/ without triggering breaking. (A lot of people pronounce "Carl" as a disyllable, but I've never heard anyone pronounce "curl" as one.)
See I analyze NAE different from you, probably because my native dialect operates differently from yours. /ɪ i/ are both monophthongs to me, not a monophthong and a diphthong. Likewise /ʊ u/ and /oʊ/ are monophthongs to me, with the following [w] in hiatus after /u o/ (
except if there was an elided intervocalic consonant!) being inserted; final /o/ can also have [w] added after it, but this is optional and may be affected by stress (e.g. I am much more likely to insert [w] after
hello than after
tomato). Of course, none of these vowels cause breaking of following /l/ for me, implying that underlyingly they are not diphthongs for me.
About /r/, the reason why I don't like "/ɝ/" is because it makes it special as rhotic vowels go, even though "/ɚ/", /ɔr/, and /jur/ (with a preceding [j]) or /ur/ (when the historical /j/ was lost) may all be realized as syllabic rhotics. So why does it get to be special and be considered its own phoneme without necessarily having a following /r/? (The reason to consider "/ɚ/" a phoneme pair is because initially it alternates with [ə(ː)ʁˤ] and it is realized with a glide as [ʁ̩ˤːʁˤ] before another vowel. You could argue that the following [ʁˤ] before a vowel is inserted, but then you would have to argue that it is inserted for all the other vowel+/r/ pairs.) Of course, I don't believe that /r/ being a glide triggers /l/ breaking for me, since I pronounce
Carl as a monosyllable.
Nortaneous wrote: ↑Sun Feb 21, 2021 6:52 pm
It's not as if true hiatus doesn't exist elsewhere in English - you still have hiatus in THOUGHT and maybe FATHER. (cawing, ...spa-ish? dunno, there's probably a better example)
I forgot about THOUGHT, which yes, I can have hiatus for. FATHER can have hiatus, but only in contrived examples (considering that morpheme-final FATHER can only exist in stressed morpheme-final syllables, and even then it is rare), and only across morpheme boundaries.
Nortaneous wrote: ↑Sun Feb 21, 2021 6:52 pm
How I see things is that unstressed intervocalic consonants in English, at least in NAE, have to be treated as distinct from both onset consonants and coda consonants; in a way they belong to both the preceding syllable and the following syllable.
This is basically reasonable, but there's probably a case for NAE just totally violating the Maximal Onset Principle.
When I try to pretend that NAE violates the Maximum Onset Principle, I end up inserting glottal stops before each unstressed vowel...
To me, how I take this is that (ignoring things like palatalization) /p t tʃ k/ each have at least three different allophones (four in the case of /t/). At the start of stressed syllables they are [pʰ tʰ tʃʰ kʰ]. After a vowel or a sonorant in a coda, they are [ʔp ʔt~ʔ ʔtʃ ʔk]. Intervocalically they are [p ɾ tʃ k]. And otherwise they are [p t tʃ k]. If these were to validate the Maximal Onset Principle, you would expect [ʔp ʔt~ʔ ʔtʃ ʔk] to be found intervocalically, but they are not, at least in NAE. Likewise, in the case of /t/ if intervocalic [ɾ] were part of a coda, one would expect it for final /t/ (which rather is [ʔt~ʔ] except after another obstruent, where it is then [t]).
Going back to /r/, though, it just seems to me that the [ʁˤ] found between vowels here is intervocalic in the same way that [ɾ] for intervocalic /t/ is. It seems strange to me to argue that /r/ itself would be special in being syllabified with the preceding vowel (whether or not that vowel is a syllabic /r/ or is merely r-colored, or (thanks to elisions, as in the case of
every) is not r-colored at all).
Nortaneous wrote: ↑Sun Feb 21, 2021 6:52 pm
I was thinking the other day about the two pronunciations of Lancaster. People who don't know how it's pronounced say something like [ˈleəŋˌkæstər]; people who do know how it's pronounced say something like [ˈlæŋkɨstər]. This [eə]-[æ] contrast happens elsewhere as well - "banker" with [æ] vs. "panko" with [eə], for example - and given Lancaster, it seems like it might be possible to avoid having either phonemic /eə/ or a contrast between /nk/ and /ŋk/ by appealing to syllabification:
[ˈleəŋˌkæstər] /læn.kæstər/
[ˈlæŋkɨstər] /lænk.ɨstər/
[ˈbæŋkər] /bænk.ər/
[ˈpeəŋkəw] /pæn.kʌw/
(I don't know how to syllabify intervocalic -st- and at this point I'd even consider a unit geminate presigmatized stop)
Intervocalic /st/ for me seems to behave somewhat like intervocalic /nt/, which has to be treated as a single intervocalic unit that belongs to both syllables (of course, intervocalic /nt/ is realized as a single consonant, [ɾ̃], most of the time, when it isn't elided).
Nortaneous wrote: ↑Sun Feb 21, 2021 6:52 pm
This requires two different nasal assimilation steps, one before and one after æ-tensing, but I think that's actually correct - the later one can fail. I heard someone today consistently pronounce "Vanguard" (the investment company) as [ˈveənˌgɑrd], with no nasal assimilation. I could also believe [ˈleənˌkæstər] and [ˈpeənˌkʌw].
(The other problem is that "panko" needs /-ʌw/ rather than /-əw/, because shouldn't it syllabify as -nk.- if the following syllable is unstressed?)
The variety here has strong nasal assimlation (as I kid I thought the
Hoan Bridge in Milwaukee was the
Home Bridge, because I had never seen its name in writing, and I naturally always heard the pronunciation where the /n/ in
Hoan assimilated to the /b/ in
bridge, but at the same time it has a clear distinction between /æ/ [ɛ~ɛə~eə] and /eɪ/ [e] before what is realized as [ŋ]. For instance, I have /eɪ/ in
banker but /æ/ in
Lancaster and
vanguard. Note that this is not a matter of productiveness, because when I make up
-ang words I naturally pronounce them with /eɪ/ but when I learn words from elsewhere, ones that I may have never heard spoken, I readily pronounce them with /æ/. Aside from being a learnèd versus non-learnèd distinction, it might have to do with stress and being polysyllabic; words that a perceive as having more than one stressed syllable seem to be more likely to be pronounced with /æ/, while short words with one stressed (typically initial) syllable are more likely to be pronounced with /eɪ/.